You are here: Home New research suggests “f” and “v” are recent speech sounds worldwide

New research suggests “f” and “v” are recent speech sounds worldwide

MPI's Dan Dediu en Scott Moisik are involved in new research published in the March 15 edition of Science magazine that could revise our understanding of the history of language.
New research suggests “f” and “v” are recent speech sounds worldwide

Dan Dediu

 

Diet-induced change in bite led to innovation in speech sounds in the world’s languages

Findings contradict theory that range of sounds remained fixed throughout human history

 

In a study titled Human sound systems are shaped by post-Neolithic changes in bite configuration, an international team led by scientists at the University of Zurich and involving researchers at two Max Planck Institutes, the University of Lyon, and Nanyang Technological University Singapore, among others, has discovered that some sounds common to modern languages likely arose only recently, as a result of diet-induced changes in the human bite. “Our findings belie the popular hypothesis that spoken languages have remained unchanged since the emergence of our species”, says Dr. Damián Blasi, co-lead author of the study with Dr. Steven Moran.

While the teeth of ancient humans once met in an edge-to-edge bite due to a tougher diet at the time, more recent softer diets allowed modern humans to retain the juvenile overbite that had previously disappeared by adulthood, with the upper teeth slightly more in front than the lower teeth and in a slight angle outwards the mouth. This shift led to the rise of a new class of speech sounds now found in half the world’s languages: “labiodentals,” or sounds made by touching the lower lip to the upper teeth. Predominant among these are “f” and “v”.

“Human speech is spectacularly diverse, ranging from ubiquitous sounds like ‘m’ and ‘a’ to the rare click consonants in some languages of Southern Africa,” remarks Dr. Moran. This range is generally thought to have been fixed by biological constraints since the emergence of Homo sapiens ca. 300,000 years ago. “However”, as senior author Professor Balthasar Bickel explains, “we underestimate the power of our biological conditions: here, we see how changes in our diet bring about changes in our bite and that this in turn shaped how modern languages sound. In Europe, for example, our data suggests that the use of labiodentals has increased dramatically only in the last couple of millenia, correlated with the rise of food processing technology such as industrial milling.”

The project was sparked when the team became intrigued by an observation made by linguist Charles Hockett back in 1985. Hockett noticed that languages which foster labiodentals are often found in societies with access to softer foods. “But there are plenty of superficial correlations involving language which are spurious, and linguistic behavior, such as pronunciation, doesn’t fossilize,” says Dr. Blasi. “We went through a real tour de force in order to evaluate this hypothesis: we combined insights, data, and methods from across the sciences to unravel and test the mechanisms underlying the observed correlations. It’s a rare case of consilience across disciplines.” What made the project possible is the availability of newly developed, large datasets, detailed biomechanical simulation models, and computationally intensive methods of data analysis, the scientists say.

“The results shed light on complex causal links between cultural practices, human biology and language,” says Dr. Moran. “They also challenge the common assumption that, when it comes to language, the past is just like the present.” Based on the findings of the study and the new methods it developed, linguists can now tackle many unsolved questions, such as how languages actually sounded thousands of years ago. Did Caesar say ‘veni, vidi, vici’ – or was it more like ‘weni, widi, wici’?

Blasi, D. E., Moran, S., Moisik, S. R., Widmer, P., Dediu, D., & Bickel, B. (2019).Human sound systems are shaped by post-Neolithic changes in bite configuration.Science, 363(6432): eaav3218. doi:10.1126/science.aav3218.

 

About MPI

This is the MPI

The Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics is an institute of the German Max Planck Society. Our mission is to undertake basic research into the psychological,social and biological foundations of language. The goal is to understand how our minds and brains process language, how language interacts with other aspects of mind, and how we can learn languages of quite different types.

The institute is situated on the campus of the Radboud University. We participate in the Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, and have particularly close ties to that institute's Centre for Cognitive Neuroimaging. We also participate in the Centre for Language Studies. A joint graduate school, the IMPRS in Language Sciences, links the Donders Institute, the CLS and the MPI.

 

 

Street address
Wundtlaan 1
6525 XD Nijmegen
The Netherlands


Mailing address
P.O. Box 310
6500 AH Nijmegen
The Netherlands

Phone:   +31-24-3521911
Fax:        +31-24-3521213
E-mail:   


Public Outreach Officer
Marjolein Scherphuis